Summary, Judgment

"Chocolate Accelerates Weight Loss!" Or, why to worry about randomization.

Legal ScholarshipAdam Chilton

In 2008, a program was launched called “Secure Communities” that sent information about anyone arrested by local police departments to the Federal Government so that their immigration status could be checked. The program had been billed as an effort to “secure” our communities by increasing immigration enforcement, and in turn, reducing crime.

In a 2014 paper, Adam Cox and Tom Miles examined whether the Secure Communities program actually reduced crime. The paper leveraged the fact that the program was rolled out gradually county by county to test its effects. The paper made a big splash because it found that increased focus on deportations wasn’t accomplishing much.

Cox and Miles’ paper wasn’t just substantively important — the research design has become influential too. Why? Their paper showed that the roll out was haphazard in a way that made it a quasi-random source of county-level variation in government policy. This kind of variation is what makes causal inference, and thus publication in peer reviewed journals, possible.

So, naturally, other scholars started using the roll out of Secure Communities to study other topics. For instance, Ariel White wrote a paper looking at the effect of Secure Communities on Latino vote turnout; Marcella Alsan and Crystal Yang looked at the effect of Secure Communities on take up of social insurance programs; Nest at al. explored the effect of Secure Communities on employment patterns for low-education native workers; and Dee and Murphy looked at the effect of Secure Communities on school enrollment. The research design was even used by Hines and Peri to study the effect of Secure Communities on crime (which, if you’re thinking that sounds an awful lot like the original Cox and Miles paper, you’d be right).

Why am I bringing this up? The Regulatory Review has been running a series of essays about the very sensible idea of trying to encourage the government to incorporate more randomization into their policy implementation. The hope is that by randomizing—like the way that the roll out of Secure Communities was staggered—it will be possible for scholars to evaluate the effect of programs in a rigorous way.

In general, I’m totally on board with this idea. Randomization makes it possible to do causal inference, and causal inference makes it possible to know if policies are working. But we do need to be worried that the proliferation of studies that will follow will start to produce bogus results. Here’s why.

As I explained in my essay for the Regulatory Review series , when researchers look for the effect of of a policy in a lot of places, it runs the risk of a problem called Multiple Hypothesis Testing (“MHT”). The concern with MHT is that statistically significant results happen randomly 5% of the time, so if we look at the effect of an intervention 20 times, we’re likely to find 1 bogus result.

My favorite example of this is the chocolate weight-loss hoax. To prove that newspapers will publish anything scientific sounding without thinking, a scientist/journalist conducted a study where people were randomly assigned to eat chocolate. The researchers then measured 18 outcomes for the people in the study. The study, predictably, found that one of the 18 variables was statistically significant thanks to random chance. An academic paper was published in a “predatory” journal based on the study, and newspapers around the world published stories about the finding with headlines like “Chocolate Accelerates Weight Loss”.

What does this problem have to do with government randomizing policy? The worry is that researchers are drawn to randomized policy interventions like moths to a flame. So when policies are randomized, people study them from every possible angle. And a lot of people looking for outcomes from the same intervention means we are naturally going to start getting some results due to the multiple hypotheses testing problem.

For instance, if studies keep looking for the effect of Secure Communities in more and more places, some of the results are going to be bogus. Not because the researchers are being nefarious, but just because of random chance.

———

If you’re interested in the topic, check out my essay and the rest of the series being published by the Regulatory Review. And shout out to Colleen Chien for writing the essay that inspired the series and inviting me to contribute. Thanks Colleen!